What’s unique about this book?
We’ve built upon the legacy of previous editions and selected more than 100 leading emotion researchers from around the world and asked them to address fundamental questions about the nature and origins of emotion, looking at what’s generally agreed upon as a field and where the jury’s still out.
What shifts have occurred in the study of emotion?
Affective scientists agree that emotions evolved, that they are more adaptive than not and that they are central features of daily life. But beyond this limited agreement, recent years have witnessed a vigorous and persistent debate about the nature of emotion with leading theorists challenging shared conceptual assumptions that has inspired and guided the field for the past quarter-century.
At the heart of this debate lies a fundamental question: What are emotions and how should we define them? Are they natural objects waiting to be discovered and catalogued (like stars) or are they constructs imposed by humans on the natural world (like constellations)? How one chooses to answer this question has clear ramifications for every other fundamental questions about the nature of emotion.
What have we learned about the science of emotion in recent years?
We’ve learned that emotion plays a central role in human experience and there is an abiding interest, among scientists, clinicians and the public at large, in understanding the nature of emotion, identifying its biological underpinnings, and determining its contribution to other psychological processes, from cognition and decision-making, to health and disease.
Over the past quarter-century, methods for eliciting, assessing and analyzing emotion have become increasingly refined and techniques for making sense of the underlying neurobiology have become more powerful and precise. The roughly 100 essays that make up the new edition of The Nature of Emotion embody many of these exciting developments and make plain the important conceptual advances that have been made since the publication of the first edition in 1994. Despite this progress, it is clear that our understanding remains far from complete.
One of the most striking developments has been the growing prominence of neuroscientific approaches to emotion or what has become popularly known as affective neuroscience. Skeptics have questioned whether neuroscience can provide conceptually important evidence, and hundreds of millions of research dollars have been spent on the assumption that it can. There is compelling evidence that studying the brain is useful for determining the nature, and not just the biological bases, of emotion.
We also know that neurobiology has been helpful for unveiling otherwise hidden features of emotion. For example, we’ve learned that reward (or pleasure) is not a single, indivisible thing, but can instead be split into wanting (appetitive motivation, craving and desire) and liking (hedonic pleasure and positive emotion). A similar story has emerged for fear, with mounting evidence that fear can be broken into two or even three more basic constituents. Beyond their implications for emotion theory (e.g., How many emotions are there? Are emotions organized into families?), these data provide new insights into both how we can better understand the underpinnings of addiction and anxiety disorders.